Quit Playing Games with My Heart

Have you seen this chart, or have you heard about the topic it describes?

(Scroll to the very bottom of the post for TL;DR if you like!)

From "On Combat", as rendered by Amazon.com. Not for commercial use.
In brief, as I have heard it taught and as this chart explains, the higher your heart rate, the worse your physical performance. This idea has implications for anyone in a physical confrontation, where an increased heart rate seems to mean an inability to perform self defense actions.

When I first encountered this concept, it did not make sense to me. In my teens I was a high school cross country and track runner, and I frequently elevated my heart rate over 200 bpm. I did not encounter these symptoms. In my twenties and thirties I played men's league ice hockey and managed to perform complex motor skills such as skating, puck handling, and shooting, all while my heart was racing. 

Now, encountering this chart and its ideas as a martial artist, I'm learning that I should be experiencing tunnel vision and racing to the bathroom when my heart rate is high. Could there be more to this phenomenon?

The chart appears in On Combat: The Psychology and Physiology of Deadly Conflict in War and in Peace by Lt. Col. David Grossman and Loren Christensen, published in 2004, but it derives from a 1997 article Grossman wrote with Bruce K. Siddle titled Psychological Effects of Combat, for the Academic Press Encyclopedia of Violence, Peace, and Conflict. 

In On Combat, the authors write:

"There is a zone that exists, generally between 115 and 145 beats per minute (bpm), where you are at your optimal survival and combat performance level... Starting at about 115 bpm, your fine-motor skills begin to deteriorate."

The authors caveat these statements:

"We should be cautious about fixing specific heart rate numbers (or any other precise measures of physiological arousal) on Condition Yellow, Red, or Black. The impact of these Conditions can vary greatly depending upon training, physical fitness, and other factors. Also, it must be understood that these heart rates apply only to survival stress or fear induced heart rate increases. You can so a set of wind sprints and get your hear rate to 200 bpm, but the effect of this exercise induced heart rate increase will not be the same as when fear or survival stress causes the increase." (emphasis added)

You can download a .pdf version of the chart with similar caveats added to the bottom.

Apparently my earlier examples involved exercise without survival stress or fear, so I did not experience the negative effects inherent in the chart. But what is the source of these statements?

The authors state later in the book: 

"The linking of specific heart rate with task performance was pioneered by Bruce K. Siddle, author of the excellent book Sharpening the Warrior's Edge, and one of the great pioneers in the field of 'Warrior Science (TM).'"

I happen to be reading Siddle's book now. On pages 48-49 he writes:

"Levitt and Gutin (1971) studied the performance of a five-choice reaction time task and found that the ideal resting heart rate performance is at the rate of 115 heart beats per minute... After the heart rate increased above 115 bpm, the subject's performance began to deteriorate, with the worst performance at 175 bpm. 

Similarly, Levitt (1972) examined the affects (sic) of various stress levels induced by exercise, on tasks varying information-processing demands... He found a clear Inverted-U effect, with optimal performance at the heart rate of 115 to 145 bpm. His students' performances were clearly less effective at heart beats of 80 and 175 bpm."

Siddle repeats these findings using different wording on page 79.

I find three flaws with the heart rate theory at this point.

First, as far as I can tell, Siddle did no research on his own, as he has no training or expertise in this area. Despite this problem, Grossman and Christensen refer to "Bruce Siddle's research" as if Siddle conducted original work. Siddle's "research" consists of citing two papers by Levitt, one of which had a co-author, Gutin. That's it. Siddle built his whole theory around two studies, and Grossman and Christensen then built then theory on top of Siddle.

Second, Grossman and Christensen's caveats noted "these heart rates apply only to survival stress or fear induced heart rate increases." I believe they added these caveats to address criticism such as those I listed earlier. However, the caveats are inconsistent with the research Siddle cites to support his heart rate theory.  Levitt's 1972 research involved subjects on treadmills, not fighters in stressful situations. According to the caveats, this exercise-induced heart rate elevation should not have caused degraded performance. However, Levitt's research did cause degraded performance. Which is it?

Third, more recent research shows that conclusions by Siddle, Grossman,Christensen are no longer tenable. For example, a 2007 paper by police officer Kathleen Vonk titled Police Performance Under Stress (pdf) stated:

"Although the Inverted-U theory is well-known within the police training arena, many essential components have unfortunately been omitted or forgotten over the years. Siddle included these components in his book Sharpening the Warrior’s Edge, however much of the information has been left behind. For example, even in basic psychology text books both task complexity characteristics and personality characteristics are mentioned as affecting one’s performance, relating these characteristics to an individualized Inverted-U. Rarely, if ever, are these mitigating factors even mentioned in defensive tactics programs...

Since human beings are so different and complex, attempting to categorize or generalize an optimal performance zone to one specific heart rate range would be virtually impossible. Rather than trying to reinvent the wheel, the police profession may be better off looking to the athletic profession in terms of improving physical and mental skills under elevated stress levels." (emphasis added)

Vonk (who has a BS in exercise science) then describes the results of six years of research where her consulting firm collected heart rate data of police officers in a variety of situations, from training to field operations. I recommend reading her paper to better understand her suggested approach, one that abandons heart rates as factors causing stress responses.

My take-away from investigating the Siddle-Grossman-Christensen heart rate theory is that you cannot make any predictions about individual performance based on heart beats per minute. Martial arts instructors should be very careful how they approach this topic with students.

Unfortunately, we cannot seem to excise this theory from popular or martial culture. For example, consider this 2013 Art of Manliness article: Managing Stress Arousal for Optimal Performance: A Guide to the Warrior Color Code. The authors of that article are also confused about Jeff Cooper's color codes, but that's a topic for another post!

If you would like to read a lengthy criticism of the heart rate theory and chart, see this blog post by police officer and trainer W. Hock Hochheim titled Death to the Heart Rate Chart. Stu Marshall, an anaesthetist (a doctor that administers anaesthesia) calls Siddle's work an urban myth

TL;DR: There is little to no science behind the theory that increased heart rates degrade performance. Multiple other factors appear to be responsible, i.e., read Vonk's paper.

What do you think of the heart rate theory? Is it bogus, benign, or beneficial? Stay informed of new blog posts by following me on Twitter @martialvitality.

Update: 16 January 2023

I continue to see references to this "high heart rate = condition black" nonsense. Here are three counter-examples based on real data and science.

First, in October 2021, NHL player Alex Killorn shared some health statistics data collected during game play. Check out his heart rate chart:


The article states "During games, Killorn’s average heart rate is usually about 130 beats per minute (including the extended breaks between periods), with a max HR of just under 180 bpm. Looking at a sample of his in-game HR data, you can see how it distinctly rises and falls in short bursts around his shifts on the ice, which only last for about 40-45 seconds."

According to the Siddle & Grossman chart, Killorn should be exhibiting "irrational flight or free, freezing, submissive behavior, voiding of bladder and bowls, [and degradation] of gross motor skills." Obviously that is not happening.

Perhaps you think because Killorn is an elite NHL player that his heart works differently? Or perhaps he's a singular superman? 

Let's look at data from members of the Polish junior national team (U18), presented in the paper Game Intensity Analysis of Elite Adolescent Ice Hockey Players. Here is a table summarizing heart rate data for 20 players collecting during game play:


Note the average (mean) heart rate maximum for forwards an defensemen was 195 bpm. Again, followers of the Siddle & Grossman view would expect these players to be helpless. The range is even more startling -- some maximum heart rates are over 210-220 bpm!

Now these are elite juniors, critics might say. Of course they can handle higher heart rates. Let's look at measurements from a middle age men's beer league. 

A 2005 article titled Dying with Their Skates On recounts the experience of a 49-year-old Canadian man who measured his heart rate while playing adult men's league hockey. 

"I did a normal warm-up, and I went from a resting heart rate of 70 beats per minute to 90," he recalls of his first self-test. "Then I played a shift and looked at the monitor and it said 188 [beats per minute]"...

"I didn't believe the monitor, so I tried it again. I got the same thing. Then a third time. Same again. After that, I figured it was accurate and I stopped looking. It was scary and I never told any of my fellow players to try it," he said of a league that comprises men from their 40s to their 60s, with a smorgasbord of occupations from garment industry executives to architects.

"They're not all in really great shape, but hockey is their form of athletics. A couple of the guys run on a regular basis, but a third of them don't work out at all in terms of staying in shape."

Mr. Shulman engaged in a little soul-salving rationalization. "That 188 was certainly above where my heart rate should be. What saves you is that it's a short burst of anerobic activity," he reasoned. (emphasis added)

Here we have a middle-aged man scoring above the "condition black" zone, and the biggest risk is that he is working too hard. There are definite health risks here, as stated in the article, but he is not suffering the degradations predicted by Siddle & Grossman.

A final criticism could be that these hockey players are not facing "life-threatening" situations, i.e., it takes a combination of sense of risk plus heart rate to cause a person to become a puddle on the ground. I'm not so sure that is the case. Hockey is a risky sport, where players check and potentially fight each other. Yes, juniors and adult beer league players do not fight, and beer league players may not check that hard, but there is still risk there. I've played in these beer leagues, and most of the fun happens after scoring a goal or when the game ends.

If I find other compelling material, I will continue to add to this post.

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Nothing But the Best for My Stuff

Retiring from Martial Arts, For Now at Least